کارکرد تعلیمی «تفکر خلاف‌واقع» در چهار کتاب قصۀ کودک: با رویکرد روایت‌شناسیِ شناختی

نوع مقاله : پژوهشی -نظری اصیل

نویسنده
کارشناسی‌ارشد، ادبیات کودک و نوجوان، دانشگاه پیام نور، تهران، ایران.
چکیده
روایت‌شناسی شناختی به مطالعۀ عوامل تداعی‌کننده و تفسیرساز متن می‌پردازد. یکی از شناختارهای تفسیرگر متن، تفکر خلاف‌واقع است. هنگامی که کنش یکی از شخصیت‌های داستان به پیامدی ناگوار یا خوشایند می‌انجامد؛ پیام اخلاقی داستان از مقایسۀ واقعیتِ موجود با سناریوی خلاف‌واقع، استخراج می‌شود. یعنی خواننده به‌طور خودکار سناریوی خلاف‌واقعی را تصور می‌کند که می‌توانست به‌جای واقعیتِ حاضر اتفاق بیفتد و مانع وقوع حادثۀ مطلوب یا نامطلوبِ موجود شود. این پژوهش با روش تحلیلی‌ـ‌‌توصیفی و با مطالعۀ موردیِ چهار کتاب قصه در گروه سنی «الف» و «ب» انجام شده است: «عموگرگه» از مجموعۀ افسانه‌های ایتالیاییِ دست اسکلت (کالوینو، 1401)، تربچه‌خانم (سرمشقی، 1394)، یخی که عاشق خورشید شد (موزونی، 1398) و بز زنگوله‌پا (شاملو، 1398). این قصه‌ها به‌صورت هدفمند و به‌خاطر انطباقشان با انواع تفکر خلاف‌واقع انتخاب شده‌اند. مقالۀ پیش‌ِرو بر آن است که با رهیافت روایت‌شناسیِ شناختی به چگونگی کارکرد تعلیمی طرحوارۀ تفکر خلاف‌واقع در قصه‌ها پاسخ دهد. این طرحواره از منظر جهت به دو جهتِ «به‌سوی بالا» و «به‌سوی پایین» و از منظر ساختار به دو ساختارِ «افزایشی» و «کاهشی» و از منظر معیار ارجاع به سه معیار «خودارجاع»، «دیگرارجاع» و «بدون ارجاع» دسته‌بندی می‌شود. پژوهش حاضر ضمن دسته‌بندی قصه‌ها براساس انواع تفکر خلاف‌واقع نشان می‌دهد جهت روبه‌بالا با رویکرد سلبی و ادبیات تعلیمی‌ـ‌تحذیری همبستگی دارد و جهت روبه‌پایین با رویکرد ایجابی و ادبیات تعلیمی‌ـ‌ترغیبی.

کلیدواژه‌ها

موضوعات


عنوان مقاله English

The Educational Function of "Counterfactual Thinking" in Four Children's Storybooks: A Cognitive Narrative Approach

نویسنده English

amir hosein Zanjanbar
Children ̓s and Young Adult Literature, Payame Noor University, Tehran, Iran.
چکیده English

Cognitive narratology examines the factors that elicit and interpret a text. One such interpretive structure is counterfactual thinking. When a story character’s action leads to either a pleasant or unpleasant consequence, the story’s moral message is extracted by comparing the existing reality with a counterfactual scenario. In other words, the reader automatically envisions a counterfactual scenario that could have occurred instead of the current reality, thereby preventing the occurrence of the existing desirable or undesirable event. This research was conducted using a descriptive-analytical method and a case study of four storybooks in age groups “A” and “B”. These books are “Uncle Wolf” from the collection of Italian Fables Hand of the Skeleton (Calvino, 2022), “Mrs. Pumpkin” (Sarmashghi, 2015), “The Ice that Fell in Love with the Sun” (Movzouni, 2019), and “The Bell-Footed Goat” (Shamlou, 2019). These stories were purposefully selected due to their alignment with different types of counterfactual thinking. The aim of this paper is to determine how the educational function of the counterfactual thinking template operates in stories, using a cognitive narratology approach. This template is categorized into two directions: “upward” and “downward”, two structures: “additive” and “subtractive”, and three criteria: “self-reference”, “other-reference”, and “non-reference”. By categorizing the stories based on the types of counterfactual thinking, this study demonstrates that the upward direction correlates with a negative approach and didactic-deterrent literature, while the downward direction correlates with a positive approach and didactic-incentive literature.

Extended Abstract

Introduction

Cognitive narratology examines the factors that elicit and interpret a text. One such interpretive structure is counterfactual thinking. Humans evaluate events and occurrences not only based on what transpired, but also contemplate how those events could have unfolded differently. In cognitive psychology, "the tendency to construct a non-real aspect for realities is called counterfactual thinking" (Kahneman & Tversky, 1982). Counterfactual thinking activates automatically, comparing the existing scenario with a possible alternative and making judgments between them. This type of thinking is divided into two directions based on orientation: “upward” and “downward”. Upward counterfactual thinking is a process where the individual compares the real situation with a more desirable counterfactual situation that could have occurred but did not, leading to dissatisfaction with the current situation and regret over the lost opportunity (Baron et al., 2009: 92). Conversely, downward counterfactual thinking imagines a scenario that, if it had transpired, would have led to worse consequences than the current situation. Based on this thinking, the individual feels satisfied with the current situation or grateful that a worse outcome did not occur.

Counterfactual thinking also manifests structurally in two ways: additive and subtractive. The additive structure recreates realities by introducing a new element to the situation. For instance, a person might imagine that if their father had undergone heart surgery, they wouldn’t have passed away. In other words, the condition for the non-occurrence of the current event was the execution of an action in the past. The subtractive structure attempts to create a different reality by eliminating elements from the situation. For example, a person might imagine that if they had not transferred the house to their son’s name, they would not now be spending the rest of their life in a nursing home.



Counterfactual thinking has three criteria of reference. An individual who constructs a counterfactual scenario can build the constructed scenario from the existing one by adding or removing an element in themselves (self-reference), in others (other-reference), or in the event itself (non-referential). For example, “If I hadn’t been speeding, I wouldn’t have hit this pedestrian” (self-referential); “If this pedestrian had used the footbridge, he wouldn’t have collided with my car” (other-referential); “If the road hadn’t been slippery, I could have controlled my car” (non-referential).



Background

The term "cognitive narratology" was first introduced by John in the article "Windows of Focalization: Deconstructing and Reconstructing a Narrative Concept" (1996); however, before him, researchers in the 1980s such as Jaus (1982), Tompkins (1980), Perry (1979), and Sternberg (1978) had paved the way for the emergence of this approach.

In the realm of Iranian children's and young adult literature, cognitive narrative research is limited to the articles "Representation of Cognitive Processes in the Story of Auntie Cockroach: Based on Discourse Analysis" (Zanjanbar and Zare, 2020) and "Cognitive Narratology of Humor in Children's Stories: A Schema-Based Approach" (Zanjanbar et al., 2021).



Objectives and Questions

This paper aims to investigate the role of the counterfactual thinking pattern in how the moral message of children's stories is represented. In this regard, by examining and comparing the mentioned stories, it seeks to answer three questions: 1. In these stories, how is the counterfactual thinking schema represented in terms of direction and structure? 2. What is the relationship between the structure of employing counterfactual thinking in the stories and their educational function? 3. In terms of employing the counterfactual thinking schema, what are the commonalities and distinctions among these stories?



Research Method

This research was conducted through a descriptive-analytical method. The case studies of this paper cover four storybooks in age groups "A" and "B": the story "Uncle Wolf" from the first volume of the Italian fables collection titled Hand of the Skeleton (Calvino, 2022), Mrs. Pumpkin (Sarmashghi, 2015), The Ice that Fell in Love with the Sun (Movzouni, 2019), and the folk tale The Bell-Footed Goat (Shamlou, 2019). These stories were purposefully selected due to their alignment with different types of counterfactual thinking.



Conclusion

Counterfactual thinking manages the reader's judgment by substituting a possible scenario. In children's stories, episodes based on counterfactual thinking are categorized into two "upward" and "downward" directions, two "additive" and "subtractive" structures, and three reference criteria of "self-reference", "other-reference", and "non-reference". Upward thinking is accompanied by a negative feeling and dissatisfaction with the occurred event. Therefore, episodes based on upward counterfactual thinking employ a deterrent teaching method and instill fear of adverse consequences. With a negative and alarmist view of outcomes, they dissuade the child from performing certain actions or behaviors. In contrast, since downward counterfactual thinking is accompanied by the reader's positive feeling and satisfaction with reality, episodes based on downward counterfactual thinking employ an encouraging and incentivizing method, guiding the child towards certain behaviors with a positive approach. While the case studies in this paper demonstrate a correlation between the direction of counterfactual thinking and the teaching method, they do not find a correlation between the additive or subtractive structure of counterfactual thinking and the presentation of educational outcomes. Furthermore, this research reveals that in terms of complexity in using the counterfactual thinking schema, stories fall into two categories: simple and composite. Simple stories are based on one type of counterfactual thinking and usually engage the reader only once at the end of the story. On the other hand, composite stories form different types of counterfactual thinking depending on various episodes. In this context, composite stories alternate between the “carrot and stick” approach in different episodes. In one episode, they use a deterrent teaching method (stick) to discourage negative actions, and in another episode, they encourage positive actions through an incentivizing method (carrot). In contrast, simple stories employ only one of the two deterrent or incentivizing teaching methods.

Regarding the reference criterion, the child constructs the self-referential counterfactual scenario based on identifying with the beloved story character, the other-referential counterfactual scenario based on other characters, and the non-referential counterfactual scenario based on chance events.

کلیدواژه‌ها English

Didactic Literature
children's story
cognitive narratology
counterfactual thinking
Barron, Robert (2009). Social Psychology. (Y. Karami; Trans). Tehran: Ravan. [in Persian].
Calvino, Italo (2022). Afsāne-hā-ye Itāliyāiy: Dast-e Eskelet. (Pia, Valentinis; Illus). (Mahyā, Bayāt; Trans). Tehran: Hoopā. [in Persian].
Dixon, P. & Bortolussi, M. (2001). Prolegomena for a science of psychonarratology. In W. van Peer & S. Chatman (Eds.) New Perspectives on Narrative Perspective, pp. 275–87. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Ghalani, Behzad (2008). Se Māhi dar Berke. (Hamed, Pazhtar; Illus). Tehran: Rāghem. [in Persian].
Hajmohammadi. Fereshteh & Hamidtaher Neshatdoust (2017). "Comparing the Counterfactual thinking in Patients with Depression, Anxiety and Healthy People". Shenakht Journal of Psychology and Psychiatry. 4(3). Pp 36- 48. [in Persian].
Herman, D. (2007). "Nonfactivity, Tellability, and Narrativity". Presentation for a Workshop on “Events, Eventfulness, and Tellability” sponsored by the University of Hamburg’s Interdisciplinary Centre for Narratology and the University of Ghent; Ghent, Belgium, February 2007.
Hirata, Shogo (2019). Little Red Riding Hood. (Shenel Ghermezi). (Ali, Ashna; Trans). Tehran: Ghadyāni. [in Persian].
Jahn, M. (1996)."Windows of Focalization:Deconstructing and Reconstructing a Narratological Concept". Style 30 (1996): 241–267.
Jahn, M. (1997). "Frames, Preferences, and the Reading of Third Person Narratives: Towards a Cognitive Narratology". Poetics Today 18(1997): 441–468.
Jahn, M. (1999). "Speak, friend, and enter’: Garden Paths, Artificial Intelligence, and Cognitive Narratology". Narratologies: New Perspectives on Narrative Analysis. (David Herman; Ed). Columbus: Ohio State Univ. Press.
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky, (Eds.), Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201-208). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Khoshouei, Mahdieh Sadat & Abolghassem Nouri (2009). "Psychometric Evaluation of the Persian Version of the Counterfactual Thinking for Negative Events Scale (CTNES)". Advances in Cognitive Sciences. 11(2). Pp 13- 23. [in Persian].
Klassen, Jon (2017, a). I Want My hat. (Cherā az man miporsi). (Maryam, Banayi; Trans). Tehran: Porteghāl. [in Persian].
Klassen, Jon (2017, b). This is not My hat. (Hich Kas Peydā-yam Nemikonad). (Maryam, Banayi; Trans). Tehran: Porteghāl. [in Persian].
Miller, D., & Turnbull, W. (1992). The counterfactual fallacy: Confusing what might have been with what ought to have been. In Lerner, M. (Ed.), Life Crises and Experiences of Loss, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 179–193.
Mahouti, Mehri (2023). "Vid… Vid" in Ghesse-hā-ye Nim-vajabi. (Neda, Azimi; Illus). Tehran: Enteshārāt-e Ghesse va Dāstān & Mer′āt.
Mohammadi, Mohammad (2015). Gorbeh-yi ke Moosh-hā rā Doost Dāsht. (Adel, Rostampour; Illus). Tehran: Kānoon-e Parvaresh-e Fekri-e Koodak va Novjavān. [in Persian].
Perry, M.( 1979). "Literary Dynamics: How the Order of a Text Creates Its Meanings (With an Analysis of Faulkner’s ‘A Rose for Emily)". Poetics Today 1(1979): 35–64, 311–361.
Roese, N. J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of personality and social psychology, 66(5), 805-818.
Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1995). Outcome controllability and counterfactual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 620-628.
Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 133-148.
Shamlou, Ahmad (2019). Boz-e Zangooleh-Pā. (Ali Reza, Asadi; Illus). Tehran: Cheshmeh. [in Persian].
Sarmashghi, Fatemeh (2015). Torobcheh Khānoom. (Marziyeh, Sarmashghi; Illus). Tehran: Elmi va Farhangi. [in Persian].
Sherman, S., & McConnell, A. (1995). Dysfunctional implications of counterfactual thinking: When alternatives to reality fail us. In Olson, J. (Ed.), What might have been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 199-232.
Sternberg, M.(1978). Expositional Modes and Temporal Ordering in Fiction. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins UP.
Tompkins, J. (1980). Reader-Response Criticism: From Formalism to Post-Structuralism. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins UP.
Zanjanbar, Amir Hossein & Hossein Zar′e (2020). Cognitive Process Representation in the Tale of "Aunt Beetle": based on Discourse Analysis. Thinking and Children. 11(1). Pp 71- 94. [in Persian].
Zanjanbar, Amir Hossein; Hossein Zar′e & Belghis Rovshan (2021). "Psychonarratology of Humor in Child Stories: A Schematic Approach". Thinking and Children. 12(1). Pp 75- 101. [in Persian].
Zarifi, Marjan (2018). "Noghteh-hā-ye Siyāh" in Mā Asb-hā rā Khasteh Kardim. Tehran: Ghoo. [in Persian].